Journal of Animal Research & Veterinary Science Category: Agriculture Type: Research Article

Communication in a Small Herd of Semi-Domestic Elephants another Interpretation of their Social Organization, Social Contract and World View

Marthe Kiley-Worthington1*
1 Centre de eco etho recherche, La Combe, Bezaudun sur Bine, Drome, France

*Corresponding Author(s):
Marthe Kiley-Worthington
Centre De Eco Etho Recherche, La Combe, Bezaudun Sur Bine, Drome, France
Tel:+33 475532027,
Email:marthekileyworthington@gmail.com

Received Date: Dec 07, 2018
Accepted Date: Jan 17, 2019
Published Date: Jan 31, 2019

Abstract

Summary

The communication between 6 individually identified free ranging young elephants were observed for over 200 hours (1278 elephant hours) in a nature reserve in Zimbabwe. 97 different behaviours, some explicit (whose meaning is clear) and some implicit (whose meaning is hidden), were recorded. Visual signals were the most common. The meaning of the 22 most common behaviours was assessed from recipient responses. Two surprising results were that (i) many behaviours were ignored by the recipients, and (ii) reciprocity was common. Correlations between the rank orders in the 4 different behavioural categories (aggression, affiliation, avoidance and interest) did not indicate an overall “dominance order”. Rather it indicated that behaviour encouraging group cohesion (showing interest and affiliation) was much more common than any related to competition in the group (that is aggression, avoiding and withdrawing). It is argued that the large number of implicit behaviours which indicate slight arousal also indicate “uncertainty”. Many behaviours ‘meanings were context dependent (i.e. the same behaviour used in a variety of contexts with its particular meaning only assessable from the context). To interpret the meaning of messages in this way, elephants must be aware of others intentions and desires, as well as others’ knowledge and roles in the society. The implication of these results for a different understanding of elephant’s social organization and mental aptitudes are discussed.

2140 interactions were recorded in 213 hours of observation (1278 elephant hours) in a group of 6 individually identifiable free ranging orphan elephants between 10 and 19 years old on a nature reserve in Zimbabwe. 97 defined behaviours were recorded, performed by the initiator or as responses. Visual signals, often associated with other sensory modalities (smell, taste, touch), were the most common. Vocalizations, audible to the human ear accounted for 0.5%, although infra-sound and seismic vibrations were not measured. The explicit and implicit meanings of the 22 most common behaviours were assessed from (i) recipient’s responses, (ii) ethological theory & (iii) folk knowledge. Many behaviours were frequently ignored by the recipient, (14 of the 22 most common behaviours) and the recipient responded reciprocally more often than expected (p<0.05). In addition, ‘interest’ in the performer, and ‘affiliation’ were more common than behaviours associated with conflict or any “dominance order” (p<0.01). Behaviours typically produced in approach/avoidance and frustration situations were common and carried a message that the communicant was “uncertain” what to do. The rank orders in the 4 categories of batched behaviours related to their normative meanings were correlated. This indicated that individual had different roles in the society, but no evidence of any overall” dominance order”. The meanings of many messages were context dependent, conveying information concerning the general level of excitement/arousal with the particular emotional state read from the context. The implication of these findings for further understanding elephants’ social contract, theory of mind, epistemology and evolutionary behaviour are discussed.

Keywords

Cognition; Elephant communication; Imitation; Social ontology theory of mind

INTRODUCTION

The elephant has the largest brain of any terrestrial mammal [1]. Since species with large brains are generally reputed to possess considerable mental development [2,3], it is likely that the brain of elephants with its large olfactory lobe, hypothalamus and cerebral cortex [4,5], possesses the hardware necessary for complex cognitive functions-including sophisticated communication, social knowledge [6], social networking or “eavesdropping” [7-12] and the development of cultures, all of which influence “intelligence” [13]. A detailed study of visual communication in particular, could further elucidate their social relations and accompanying mental attributes.

It has been widely believed that human language has an over powering influence on cognitive [14-17]. As a result, many studies have either focused on assessing whether species can learn to use an artificial human-type, context-independent language [18-22]. Anderson [23], or testing if these species have human mental characteristics such as manipulating and deceiving [6].

Communication in elephants, like that of almost all behaviours of mammals, is flexible and adaptable [24,25]. The specific meaning of the message conveyed by a particular behaviour may be independent of the context in which it occurs, that is “context independent”, similar to much of human language. But it may be “context dependent”, where the meaning conveys the state of arousal of the communicant, but the detailed meaning is read from the context [26,27].

Another feature of elephant’s communication is that it often multi-sensory. A summary of how the different senses are used in communication follows. 

Vocal communication has received most attention in a range of species including elephants (e.g. birds, whales, see forthcoming review on audition in mammals). Their auditory sensitivity ranges from infra sound to higher frequencies than humans [28]. Their vocalisations have been studied since the 1960’s [24,25, 27,29,30-32] and a number of studies have focused on the use of ultra-sound by elephants to communicate over several kilometers [33-37] identification of individuals by their vocalizations [33,38,39] and communication by sensitivity to seismic movements has also been demonstrated [40-42].

Olfactory and gustatory communication are often linked but have received less attention in elephants, although they have a very large olfactory sensitive area in the trunk, taste buds on the tongue and around the mouth, and a relatively large olfactory bulb in the brain [1,6,43], mentions that they receive and analyze a great deal of olfactory information which is important to them. The vomeronasal organ attached to the base of the brain is considered to be for the careful investigation of strong, pertinent or unfamiliar smells [44]. It is uniquely large in the elephant and has paired palatal ducts, a folded epithelium and a dorso-medial position of the neuro-epithelium indicating its complexity and links to the cerebral cortex [45], and elephants perform flehmen which is associated with the vomeronasal gland [46]. They have cutaneous glands around the temple and inter-digital glands that produce steroids as well as other secretions and have been found to secrete pheromones related to sexual status. Olfactory research has concentrated on sexual pheromones [45,47-50], but elephants have sweat glands well distributed over the body [51,52], which may produce individual smells. The multi-sensory trunk acts as a hand, a nose and a mouth thus multi-sensory information is inevitably acquired. Taste plays a major role in the choice of diets of course, and frequent tasting another (see below), is likely to be important in communication, but has been little investigated to date.

Some tactile communication has been studied with the salience of the trunk demonstrated [53-55]. Elephants are capable of extraordinarily complex manipulations with the trunk; within the blind area (see below), by touching and presumably smelling, they can sort beans that they wish to eat from those that they do not (personal video), or find rings in piles of hay with their trunks (Circus Knie). Communication by touching different parts of each other’s bodies also are important [53], but such movements also always have visual communicative cue.

Visual Communication has not been very carefully investigated in detail, although explicit trunk movements have been recorded. The first step is to study what they can see. First, they have dichromatic vision, like some colour blind humans [56], but further research is required. Due to the position of the eyes on either side of the large head, elephants have two large simultaneously observed monocular visual fields which enable him/her to acquire visual cues from a large area (300°). But they have a relatively small binocular visual field, approximately 25-60°, compared to the 100-120° of humans. The breadth of the head with the trunk protruding directly in front of the laterally placed eyes ensures that s/he has a blind spot of approximately 2m directly in front of the head [57,58]. Both the placing of the eyes, and the two simultaneous monocular visual field, presents a different visual appreciation of the world from humans’ relatively small monocular, but large binocular visual field.

It has been maintained that elephants have poor vision [51,59,60], although visual signals, such as some trunk movements [24,25,60,61] and head, tail and ear movements [24-26,29,57], have been described. Some visual displays associated with must [62] and female reproduction has also been described although these visual cues may have a biochemical foundation [63].

It is well known that elephant as well as other domestic animals react to slight visual cues indicating intentions that are emotional states. They also show an ability to imitate others [58,64], indicating perhaps emotional contagion [65,66]. These abilities have been used for centuries [67], by their handlers. Thus, such critically assessed information or “folk knowledge” must be part of any good scientific investigations, if we wish to progress in our understanding [68-71], rather than just “discovering” what is already known.

One of the objects of this paper is to report the explicit behaviours in communication (the behaviours whose meanings are clear) and to investigate the responses they cause. The implicit behaviours which have often been ignored in scientific reports, (although their meaning is often known by those who deal with elephants) have also been recorded. First, both the explicit and the implicit behaviours must be defined, their meanings critically evaluated and their involvement with other senses recognized.

Studies of explicit behaviours related to competition between individuals and this often comes with a commitment to outlining a “dominance hierarchies” [72-74]. The inclusion of both afflictive and implicit behaviours in communication might suggest different interpretations of elephant’s social contract.

Non vocal signals are unlike vocalization in their communicative value because vocalizations are only made to communicate [29]. But, visual, olfactory, taste and tactile messages may have other functions and be performed without intent to communicate, even though a message concerning their emotional state may be transferred to others watching. For example when one elephant sees another putting his trunk out to smell a strange object, he receives the message that “there is something interesting there”, even though the behavior was probably performed from curiosity, rather than with intent to communicate. Thus, the communicative meaning of many messages can be complex.

For many movements and vocalizations, the meaning may act as an analogue system indicating the general level of arousal of the animal so the same behavior may occur in a variety of contexts [26,27,75]. The behavior itself does not convey why s/he is in this state. For example, the elephant shakes his/her head when s/he (i) has an irritation on the head, (ii) is scared, (iii) about to attack, (iv) excited, (v) anticipating something (vi) cannot obtain a desire goal. In essence, the animal indicates that it is aroused, but “uncertain” what to do next. This has been demonstrated for many vocalisations, including the elephant rumble [27], head and tail movements in ungulates, elephants and carnivores [26,29,30]. The cause of the arousal can be traced to frustration or approach avoidance conflict [26,30,54,76-80].

Whether the individual is interested, aggressive, and friendly or is experiencing other emotions such as curiosity, fear or embarrassment [81], will be read from the context. Thus, a better understanding of visual signals and the consequent mental aptitudes could further illuminate the elephants’ social contract.

Undeniably, such a study is difficult. It requires experienced observers who attend to details, are inter-observer reliable, familiar with the species and can recognize individuals. Different combinations of behaviours may be performed simultaneously so some behaviour may be missed. Although not all the olfactory messages, seismic vibrations or infra-sounds can be perceived by the human observer without special equipment, if information has been transferred it is likely that there will be some visual cues.

To date, there has been no quantitative analysis of the meaning of many of the visual messages conveyed by elephants, and the importance of their trunk for manipulation and multi-sensory communication needs further study although see Kiley-Worthington and Plotnick et al., [61,82]. As Langbauer et al., mention “more work needs to be done to elucidate the specific role of many signals”.

AIMS OF THE STUDY

These are:- 
(1) To assess the importance and multi-sensory role of visual signals in elephant communication 
(2) To ascertain which behaviours are most common, both from the initiators (performers) and as responses.
(3) To help derive the meaning of different behaviours from the analysis of responses
(4) To suggest meanings for implicit visual messages
(5) To correlate the rank orders between the different behaviours and outline the implications of these results founder standing elephant social ontology, their social contract and mental aptitudes
(6) To consider the evolutionary importance of this.

Elephant communication shown in figure 1.

Figure 1: Visual gestures as three elephants begin to walk down a path.

METHODS

Subjects

A group of six semi-domestic African elephants (Loxodonta africana), five males (aged 12-19 years, all pre-musth) and one female (aged 13 years) were studied. All individuals were orphaned between 4 and 8 years old due to their mothers being shot in parts of Zimbabwe over a period of 6 years. The orphans had been captured and transported to Imire Game Ranch, Wedza. On arrival they were handled by humans and often bottle fed. After 6 months to 1 year, each was gradually introduced to the growing social group.

Husbandry and daily routine

From 6pm-7am, the elephants were kept shackled by one hind leg approximately 5m apart so they could see and smell but not touch each other in a designated area, close to human accommodation (this was to protect them from poachers and to prevent them from plundering crops). Each was given 15-20kg of hay and generous bedding. None of the elephants were observed performing stereo types or other signs of prolonged distress during this study. At 6am each elephant was mucked out by a handler. The handler then spent up to 30 minutes interacting with that elephant using a restricted vocabulary of 100 words, teaching them the names for part of their bodies and to perform simple tasks to a word command with positive reinforcement of food and verbal praise, and non-violent negative reinforcement (voice & gestures). The handlers did not have an ankus, so there could be no prodding or hurting the elephant during this process. These exercises were designed to improve the handling and teaching in an interactive and co-operative way [58]. Each elephant was fed 3kg of ‘game nuts’ after the training session had ended. At 7am the elephants were unshackled and walked to a nearby area where the handler and tourists would mount them. They would be ridden for up to 30 minutes on different paths through the reserve. Not all elephants were ridden every day - those that were not, were left to wander around and browse until the others returned. The entire group of elephants was then released into the 500ha wild life park with indigenous vegetation & other wild and domestic herbivores. They remained under surveillance by the handlers who kept between 30 and 200m from them for the rest of the day. Surveillance was necessary to prevent the elephants plundering the commercial crops (tobacco and maize), wandering outside the park or being killed by poachers. At midday they were moved to a meeting point together with 7 black rhino (Diceros bicornis) who had also been orphaned and were being kept to establish a breeding group [83]. At the luncheon site, there was a lake and they could be observed by tourists from 30 to 200m distant. From approximately 2pm-5pm, the elephant again moved around freely within the park grazing or browsing and at 5pm they were herded or ridden back to their night-time accommodation, fed hay and bedded down for the night at around 6 pm. The group typically remained relatively close by choice (average distance of nearest neighbor was 30m. [64].

Observations, training of observers & inter-observer reliability

Observations of the social interactions between all the elephants were conducted between January 6thand February 28th 1996 during the day when they were free to wander around and feed in the nature reserve (between approximately 7.30am-5pm).The observations were conducted by the author, her assistant and four of the experienced handlers who had been with the elephants for more than 6 months. No behaviours were recorded when the handlers were nearer than 3m, or the elephants attention was directed at humans rather than other elephants. The observers were given 5 days training, the ethogram shown in table 1 with the definitions of each activity, a stop watch and a tape recorder. The handlers only recorded observations once the author was confident that they were able to do so comprehensively and with parity. Each observer was checked during each session to ensure that all were clear about the definitions for each activity, used only this, and was careful to observe all the behaviours of the performer and responder. Since the elephants were accustomed to the continual presence of the human handlers between 30-200m, it was unlikely that the observer’s presence had a significant influence on the elephants’ interactive behaviour during the observational periods.

Behaviour

Definition

1. Ignore

Make no response to behaviour directed at the recipient for 20 secs afterwards.

2. Approach /make contact

Walk directed towards another for >5steps to within 5m

3. Contact activity

Stay within 1m for at least10sec while walking/running

4. Contact break

Walk away from another for at least 10m

5. Turn body towards

Whole body and head turned towards another and take at least 3 steps.

6. Follow

Follow an individual for at least 10m

7. Turn head towards another

Turn head only towards another

8. Trunk extend towards another

Trunk held out towards another for at least 5 secs.

9. Tail wag

Slower sideways movement of tail when no obvious flies were around

10. Tail swish

Rapid move of the tail sideways at least 5 times

11. Tail up

Tail raised up above the horizontal and held for at least 5sec

12. Tail withdraw

Tail pulled tight over anus

13. Flap both ears forwards & back

Move ears back and forth at least 3 times.

14. Both ears back

Ears compressed against the head with visible muscle contraction

15. Ears pricked

Both ears forward, attention directed at something held for at least 10sec

16. Swing trunk

The hanging trunk is swung back & forth or right & left at least 5 times.

17. Trunk up

Trunk raised above head

18. Trunk down & relaxed

Trunk hanging usually on ground

19. Trunk forward investigating object

Trunk held forward to smell the object of attention better

20. Puff

Air released from trunk with a puffy noise, less abrupt & loud than “blow”

21. Blow

Sudden blast of air from trunk giving rise to sudden abrupt noise.

22. Rumble

Low amplitude and pitch rumbling vocalisation

23. Trumpet

Loud higher amplitude vocalisation with changing pitch

24. Head shake

Lateral shaking of the head at least 3 times.

25. Head nod

Up and down movement of the head at least 3 times

26. Head throw

Trunk and head thrown up and sideways at least 1 time

27. Head up

Head held up high above temple for at least 5 sec

28. Walk

Lateral 4 time walk of at least 5 paces

29. Run

Diagonal or 4x fast walk for at least 5 paces

30. Lie on sternum

Four feet spread back and forwards with weight on sternum

31. Lie flat

Lying on side with legs laterally on the ground

32. Getting up

Front feet out and heave self up

33. Wallow

Lye down and move around in water or mud

34. Urinate

Eliminating urine for at least 10sec

35. Defecate

Eliminating faeces from the anus

36. Fart

Air released through the anus making a noise

37. Sigh

Slower than normal large breathe taken in and slowly released

38. Chew

Moving lower jaw up and down at least 5 times

39. Paw

Moving front leg backwards and forwards touching the ground

40. Swing leg

Lifting a front or hind leg and swinging it back and forward in the air at least 3x

41. Raise leg front

Holding a leg in the air for at least 5 secs

42. Raise or rest hind leg

Resting one leg with weight on the other hind

43. Stamp front or hind leg

Lift leg up and down hard and faster than normal

44. Increase postural tonus

Head, tail up stand erect

45. Decrease postural tonus

Head tail and whole body relaxed

46. Tusks pushing into ground

Kneeling and pushing tusks into earth, moving head

Touch/scratch/ smell self

 

47. Rub head on object

Head against object and rubbing up and down at least 3 times

48. Trunk to own mouth

 

49. Trunk to own penis

 

50. Trunk to own foot

 

51. Scratch or rub self with trunk head & eyes

Trunk twisted to rub the different area

52. Trunk on shoulder or ears,

 

53. Trunk between legs

 

54. Trunk to feet or legs rubbing/investigating

 

55. Any other area of body

 

56. Cross legs

Stand with front or hind legs crossed

57. Erection of penis

 

Scratch/smell/touch other

Recorded if trunk or parts of their bodies touch

58. Trunk into others mouth

Trunk enters open mouth of other and remains in for more than 5sc

59. Touch neck of other with trunk

 

60. Touch head of other with body or trunk

 

61. Touch trunk of other

 

62. Entwine trunks

Twist trunks around each other

63. Nibble other’s body with trunk

Trunk touching and nibbling with prehensile lips any part of another 5sc<

64. Touch leg

 

65. Touch quarters of other

 

66. Touch other’s penis

 

67. Touch between legs other

 

68. Rest chin on another

Chin rested with trunk often over body

69. Trunk smelling other

Not closer than 10cm smelling particular area

70. Trunk pushing at another’s face or thrown forward

Either trunk pushing or thrown at face of other

71. Smell/taste urine of other

Trunk either in urine or smelling urine on ground

72. Rub body on another

Rubbing body against any part of another’s body

73. Touch tail of other with trunk

 

74. Smell vulva of other with trunk

 

75. Smell penis of other with trunk

 

76. Mount another

 

Other behaviour to another

 

77. Tasting others urine

Trunk to own mouth after being in others urine.

78. Stand over another

Standing over another lying down for at least 10 sec

79. Push another with head or tusks

Head or tusks pushing another’s quarters or side

80. Flehmen

Raising trunk and testing urine probably with vomeronasal gland

81. Turning back on other

Turning face away from the other

82. Grab tail of another

Trunk holds the tail of another

83. Circle

Walk around another in a semi circle

84. Barge another

Walk fast or run at and push another

85. Watch other

Watching another carefully either with one eye or both for at least 10 secs.

86. Social Play

Touching, pushing, sparing, chasing each other without intention to hurt.

87. Object Play

Lifting/throwing/ pushing etc object

88. Push another’s head

With head or tusks

89. Tusks to tusks

Pushing twisting tusks to tusks

90. Tusks to another part of body

Tusking any other part of body of other

91. Tusk to legs of other

 

Avoid or withdraw from another

 

92. Turn away

Turn away from another for at least 5 sec

93. Turn and walk away

Turn and walk at least 5m

94. Back off

Back away from another at least 5m

Aggressive acts

 

95. Chase

Run towards another head up at least 5 m

96. Push tusks into another’s body

Anywhere on the body other than to tusks

97. Tusk to tusk pushing

 

Table 1: The behaviours recorded and their definitions.

Statistical method

Simple descriptive analysis was conducted using Mini-Tab v13.2 statistical package. A Continuous Recording, Instantaneous Time Sampling method [84], was used. All of the elephants were observed for the entire observation period with an observer for each elephant. Thus there was continual observation of all six elephants and their interactions. 97 different defined behaviours were recorded (Table 1). Some behaviours were not frequent enough to be quantitatively analysed, so much of the detailed analysis was confined to the 22 most common behaviours (Table 2).

Behaviour

No. Times Exhibited by Performer. P

No. Times Exhibited by a Recipient as a Response. R

Total Number of Times Exhibited

% of Total

Approach/contact make

677

59

736

9.9

Ignore

0

712

712

9.6

Tail wag

220

309

529

7.1

Contact activity (walk, stand or lie)

335

148

483

6.5

Flap ears

210

216

426

5.7

Contact break (general)

150

256

406

5.4

Swing trunk

182

108

290

3.9

Turn body towards

111

92

203

2.7

Tusk - to- tusk

103

96

199

2.7

Trunk up

103

78

181

2.4

Trunk to mouth

121

54

175

2.3

Follow

145

28

173

2.3

Turn head towards

75

46

121

1.6

Trunk extend

74

47

121

1.6

Head shake

61

51

112

1.5

Tusk to body

77

10

87

1.2

Both ears back

23

62

85

1.1

Entwine trunks

37

41

78

1.0

Head nod

39

36

75

1.0

Ears pricked

38

33

71

1.0

Blow

30

39

69

1.0

Tail up

27

39

66

1.0

Table 2: The 22 most common performed behaviours.

For each social interaction the identities of the initiator/performer and the recipient were recorded and all of the behaviours exhibited by both (up to 5 behaviours might be simultaneously performed). The behaviours were recorded either on dicta phones and then transferred directly into the Mini-Tab statistical package for analysis, or a psion event recorder. Additional observations of maintenance behaviour and proximity relations exhibited by the elephants were also noted every 15 minutes using a focal, scan sampling method which are reported elsewhere [64].

The behaviours recorded and their definitions are shown in table 1.

RESULTS

A total of 2410 interactions between the 6 elephants were observed over 213 hours, (that is 1278 elephant hours). This represents 1.89 interactions/elephant/hour.

The analysis of the meaning of the messages were restricted to the 22 most frequent behaviours, selected on the total number of times performed regardless if by an initiator or a recipient. These behaviours accounted for 73% of the recorded behaviours. Table 2 lists them and their frequencies.

The use of the different sensory modalities in the interactions

The frequency of the use of the different sensory modalities is shown in table 3, followed by further details for each sensory modality.

Sensory Modality

Number of Different Behaviours

Frequency

% of Total (7542) Different Behaviours Observed

Visual only, as far as could be assessed

36

4307

58%

Visual, tactile/gustatory and olfactory

29

1314

18%

Visual & olfactory

7

1553

21%

Auditory - vocal 
Auditory - non vocal

3
2
Total=77

37
111 
Total=7344

0.5%
1.5% 
Total=99%

Other behaviours whose sensory modality was not clear

27

143

1%

Table 3: The frequency of the use of the different sensory modalities.

Olfactory communication

The visual evidence of smelling included frequent movements of the trunk searching and investigating strange or interesting smells and possibly tastes 39% of all of the behaviours recorded in this study had an olfactory component: (18% visual, tactile, gustatory and olfactory, and 21% visual and olfactory). There may have been other olfactory messages without visual cues which the observers could not pick up.

Gustatory communication

It was not possible to completely separate taste from olfactory or tactile cues, but, there was a taste component in 18% of the interactive behaviours (Table 3).

Tactile communication

Tactile and gustatory cues could not be separated visually either as when touching, the trunk may also be tasting and smelling. The elephants also touched each other with other parts of the body. There was a tactile component in 18% of all the behaviours performed

Auditory communication

This was used very little in this group (0.5% of observations). It is possible that during the study, the elephants were using infra sound or seismic vibrations, but, visual signs would be likely to have been seen.

There are also a variety of non-vocal noises made by elephants without the larynx. These include blows, sighing, sniffing and puffs, stamping, breaking sticks and so on. Blow and puff accounted for 1% of the behaviours used in social interactions. Environmental noises made by individuals were not recorded.

Visual communication

There was a visual component in 97% of the behaviours recorded (Table 3: 58%+21%+18%). Even though other sensory cues were sometimes attached, over half (58%) had only visual cues as far as we could ascertain.

The use of the trunk

Sixteen different trunk movements were distinguished that were not related to feeding, although some of these were relatively rare (Table 4). These trunk movements accounted for 18.1% of all the interactive behaviours. 

Trunk Movement

No. Times Exhibited by Performer, P

No. Times Exhibited by Recipient, R

Total Number of Times Exhibited, P+R

% of 7542 Behaviours

Swing trunk

182

108

290

3.9

Trunk up

103

78

181

2.4

Trunk to another’s mouth

121

54

175

2.3

Trunk extend to another

74

47

121

1.6

Trunk to vulva

72

15

87

1.2

Trunk to penis of other

56

29

85

1.1

Entwine trunks

37

41

78

1.0

Trunk to body of other

52

12

64

0.9

Trunk to face of other

34

29

63

0.8

Trunk to anus of other

17

43

60

0.8

Trunk to head/shoulder of other

35

23

58

0.7

Trunk to tail of other

36

6

42

0.5

Trunk to own mouth

19

19

38

0.5

Trunk to rear of another

12

1

13

0.2

Trunk down

4

4

8

0.1

Trunk to neck of other

7

1

8

0.1

Total

861

510

1371

18.1%

Table 4: The Use of the Trunk in communication. The different movements, their frequencies and the percentage of all the behaviours recorded.

Further discussion on the use of the trunk is below.

Analysis of the meanings of different behaviours

When studying communication, as a rule, only behaviours with an explicit message are recorded, such as aggressive (e.g. fighting with the tusks, barging and tusking another, trunk pushing) and withdrawal and avoiding. Affinitive behaviour such as touching or rubbing another, are explicit, but have less frequently been recorded. Showing interest in another is explicit, but has rarely been recorded. There are much other behaviour used in interactions which have an “implicit” or hidden meaning which we will unravel. Meanings can be accessed from an analysis of the recipient’s responses, but this is not always clear, as the responses vary. Nevertheless, it can be the first step. Table 5 shows the three most common responses to the 22 most frequently performed behaviours.

Behaviour Performed

Number of Times Recorded

Number of Different Types of Responses

Most Common Response

Second Most Common Response

Third Most Common Response

Approach/contact make

690

76

Ignore310

Contact break106.

Tail wag69

Tail wag

359

68

Ignore73

Tail wag 66**

Contact31

Contact activity (walk, stand or lie)

339

56

Ignore137

Contact 117**

Tail wag49

Flap ears

335

59

Ignore73

Flap ears 66**

Tail wag54

Contact break (general)

80

39

Ignore 60

Tail wag11

Follow7

Swing trunk

217

50

Ignore88

Tail wag34

Swing Trunk 29**.

Turn body towards

161

52

Ignore 40

Tail wag27

Flap ears18

Tusk - to- tusk

198

35

Tusk-to-tusk 84**

Entwine trunks18

Ignore10

Trunk up

169

57

Ignore29

Trunk up 14**

Tail wag11

Trunk to own mouth

141

44

Ignore41

Contact break20

Trunk to own mouth 13**

Trunk extend

104

40

Ignore19

Flap ears12

Tail wag12

Entwine trunks

79

21

Entwine trunks 28**

Tusk-to-tusk17

Contact break4

Tusk to body

55

37

Contact break24

Ignore14

Leap away13

Both ears back

45

28

Tail wag6

Flap ears4

Contact break 3

Head nod

56

26

Ignore 17

Tail wag10

Head nod 8**

Ears pricked

58

25

Ignore 14

Prick ears 8**

Tail wag8

Blow

74

35

Tail wag8

Flap ears5

Trunk to mouth5

Tail up

59

26

Ignore 28

Tail wag 5

Ears back 4

TOTAL number
Responses in category.

3467

826

1051

554

348

Table 5: The number of times each of the 22 most common behaviours were performed, the number of different behaviours elicited, and the 3 most common responses.
Note: Bold=Behavior ignored by recipient. Dyadic movements by definition, excluded from analysis. Italic: Are reciprocal **=t test significant at P<0.01

Approach, the most commonly exhibited behavior (690 times), caused 76 different behavioural responses (Table 5). All the behaviours had a number of different responses (column 3).

The implicit behaviour, tail wagging or swishing, was the second most common response. It was often performed with other behaviours (33.2%). See below for discussion.

Ignore as a response

Curiously, the most common response was to ignore the directed behavior, defined as “no observable change of behaviour exhibited by the recipient within one minute of the performer’s directed behaviour” (see Table 1). All the behaviours that were not of their nature dyadic (e.g. tusk to tusk, trunk entwine) were ignored at some point, a total of 712 times (20.5%). Table 6 shows the most and least commonly ignored behaviours.

Performed Behaviour

Number of Times Ignored

Total Number of Times this Behaviour is Performed

Percentage Each Behaviour is Ignored

Most often ignored (more than 20% of occasions performed)

Contact break (general)

310

677

60%

Follow

73

220

50.3%

Approach/contact make

137

335

45.8%

Head nod

73

210

43.6%

Contact activity (walk, stand or lie)

102

150

40.9%

Flap ears

88

182

37.6%

Ears pricked

40

111

36.8%

Turn body towards

10

103

36%

Swing trunk

29

103

35.7%

Turn head towards

23

121

34.7%

Tail wag

73

145

33.2%

Head shake

26

75

32.8%

Tail up

8

27

29.6%

Trunk up

19

74

28.2%

Trunk extend

20

61

25.7%

Least commonly ignored (less than 20% of occasions when performed)

Trunk to own mouth

14

77

19%

Tusk to body of other

3

23

18%

Blow

4

37

16.7%

Both ears back

17

39

13%

Table 6: The most and least commonly ‘ignored’ of the 22 most frequent behaviours.

The less ignored behaviours are likely to be those with the strongest messages. These include: (i) putting the trunk into his own mouth. (ii) Tusking another’s body, which caused a leap away, but could also be ignored? The least ignored of all were “blow” and “both the ears back”. The most common responses to both were tail wagging, and/or putting the trunk in his own mouth (Table 5).

Why so many behaviours are frequently ignored is discussed below.

Do as done by, imitation or reciprocity (Table 5 italic)

Many responses were reciprocated significantly more than expected (Table 5 in italic). In fact, reciprocal/imitative behaviours were one of the three most frequent responses for 7 of the 19 most common behaviours (Table 5 and details Table 7). All except for “blow” and “tusk to body of the other” were significantly reciprocated (Chi2, P<0.01).

Performed Behaviour

Total Times Elicited by Performer, Frequency

Same Behaviour Elicited by Recipient, Frequency

Same Behaviour Elicited by Recipient, %

Value of Chi2

P Value

Contact activity
(walk, stand or lie)

335

117

34.9

13.96

0.001

Flap ears

210

66

31.4

31.08

0.001

Tail wag

220

66

30.0

29.87

0.001

Ears pricked

38

14

21.1

16.02

0.001

Head nod

56

8

20.5

9.03

0.001

Swing trunk

182

29

15.9

15.16

0.001

Head shake

92

8

8.7

6.61

0.025

Trunk up

169

14

8.3

8.46

0.005

Tail up

27

3

11.1

22.45

0.001

Trunk to mouth

121

13

10.7

10.33

0.001

Both ears back

23

2

8.7

11.08

0.005

Blow

30

2

6.7

1.69

NS

Turn head towards

75

3

4.0

5.01

0.025

Contact break (general)

150

5

3.3

5.82

0.025

Tusk to body

77

2

2.6

1.09

NS

Approach/contact make

677

17

2.5

9.92

0.001

 

 

 

 

 

 

Table 7: “Do-as-done-by”, reciprocity or imitation by responder.

Why reciprocity is so common is discussed below.

Batching the behaviours according to their meaning

The meaning of explicit behaviours is evident. Here they are batched into 3 categories, 1) those related to aggression and avoidance, 2) affiliation and 3) showing interest in the other. 

Much implicit behaviour are originally related to skin irritation, such as tail wagging, head nodding or shaking, scratching, and ear flicking (further discussion below). These behaviours were frequent, but their meaning in social interactions is not obvious. These results indicate that these behaviours show some arousal because of indecision/ uncertainty of what to do (approach or avoid: frustrated). Therefore, they are placed in the 4th category labeled “uncertain” or indecision. Category 5 is behaviours that occurred too in frequently to analyze.

To summarize, the batched categories are:-

Socially positive behaviours

Interest/Attention that is turning the attention to the other and affiliative behaviours. Socially positive behaviours foster group cohesion: “sticking” together. (a) Affiliation, friendly behavior (24.27%), significantly higher than expected (Chi2, P<0.01), and (b) interest (22.94%). These 2 categories accounts for 47.66% of all the behaviours recorded, almost half.

Socially negative behaviours

Aggression and also avoid and withdraw-Socially negative behaviours relate to unpleasantness to another, its avoidance or prediction (such as avoiding and withdrawing). Socially negative behavior promotes group division or “splitting”. Socially negative behaviours accounted for 18.4%, significantly less than expected (Chi2 p<0.05).

Uncertainty or indecision

These are frequently performed behaviours that often occur during frustration or approach-avoidance conflict which indicates some arousal, usually caused by indecision. Interestingly, this category was the most common: 29.27%, significantly more than expected (Chi2, P<0.01).

Unclassified behaviours

The communicative meaning of these behaviours remains unknown due to their infrequent performance.

The behaviors in each category are listed in table 8. Table 9 shows the frequency that the different categories were recorded in the interactions.

Interest

Affiliation

Aggression and Avoidance

Uncertainty

Unclassified

Approach
Ears forward pricked
Ears back
Look at
Trunk extend to
Tail up
Puff
Turn body towards
Turn head towards
Walk faster
Trot
Circle another
Trunk up
Smell other from a distance

Rest head/neck on other
Rumble
Contact make
Contact walk
Contact stand
Stand over
Follow
Grab others tail
Mount with or without erection
Rub against another
Rub head against other,
Smell & touch other
Smell penis or vulva,
Touch anus, penis, vulva, face, body, neck, nose, shoulder, tail
Nibble head, body
Playing
Entwine trunks
Trunk to others mouth

Barge into other
Push another’s body or tum
Tusk to head, rear or body of the other
Tusk to tusk
Throw trunk at, or smack with trunk 

Ears Flap
Head nod
Head throw
Head shake
Chew
Tail wag & swish Blow
Paw & Stamp
Raise leg
Swing leg
Trunk to own mouth
Swing own trunk

Rest leg
Lie down
Urinate
Defeacate
Graze
Dust throw
Walk
Pull tree & eat leaves.

Table 8: The behaviours batched in the five different categories relating to their meaning.
 

Batched Behaviour

Total Performed
P

% Total Performed

Total Recipient Responses RR
(excluding ignore)

% of Recipient Responses Recorded

Total
% of
P+RR
P+RR

Affiliative

1569

30.51**

1018

21.47

2587

24.27

Interest

1341

26.77

1104

21.24

2445

22.94

Uncertainty

1301

25.3

1818**

34.98

3119

29.27**

Aggression

808

15.1

368 -*

0.7

1176

11.2

Avoid

123*

2.3

648

1.24

771

7.2

Unclassified

403

7.26

136

0.26

539

5.03

TOTAL

5545

100

5092

100

10,637

99.91

Table 9: The frequency of each behavior in the 5 categories either performed by the initiator or as a response.
Note: *=significant at p>0.05; **=significant at p>0.01

DISCUSSION

Comparative Sociality in mammals measured from the frequency of their interactions

A comparative measure of the “sociability”, that is the frequency that a species communicates with its co specifics, has been neglected. This can be represented as the number of interactions per animal per hour observed. There were 1.89 interactions/elephant/hour, which is more than in horses: 1.01/horse/hour when the data was recorded and analyzed in the same way [85]. Whether other elephants are such frequent conversationalist, remains to be explored.

Protective responses

In many canids, felids and ungulates, the origin of some behaviours such as pulling the ears back, appears to be related to protection. This can be ritualized and become a signal indicating defensive threat. Ear withdrawal is particularly obvious and ritualized in species without horns such as canids, felids, equids and camelids. Here, the ears are drawn back and the orifices rotated towards the head [29]. The large aural orifices of elephants are not possible to protect in this way, but the ears themselves can be pulled back and flattened against the side of the head which may protect them from being torn in dangerous situations. Forty five different behaviours were elicited when an initiator put both his/her ears back, and it was often reciprocated (see Table 5) and accompanied by tail wagging, indicating uncertainty. The third most frequent response to ears back was to withdraw. The meaning of ear flattening is suggested below.

The use of the different sensory modalities in communication

The most frequent behaviours recorded in interactions were visual (97%). However many visual cues have inherent tactile, olfactory or gustatory involvement. This is the case for any trunk movements which inevitably involve the possibility of smell (and sometimes touch and taste) as well as visual positional messages. Olfactory and tactile communication is important in elephant communication, but they generally have a visual component and consequently also display a visual message. Vocal messages, audible to the human ear, or with visual indicators (for example trunk vibrations), were relatively rare in this group, although it is possible that seismic or infrasound messages were not picked up.

Olfactory communication

Although research attention has been placed on the production of pheromones from particular glands (see introduction) most of elephant’s olfaction may be to acquire diverse environmental and/or social information. With their well developed olfactory organs and areas for its analysis in the brain information can probably be gathered from the wind, changes in air humidity, smells of vegetation such as rising sap, potential fruiting or flowering and the recognition of individual plants, buds and leaves.

Information could be acquired in this way about both the past and the future from smells of seasonal changes. For example, Namibian elephants trek around 150km across the desert to baobab trees when they are fruiting, so they must know when this is happening perhaps partly by recognizing smells of seasonal changes. In social contexts, the age and sex of an individual can be recognized from urine or faces and elephants must be able to recognize individual elephants as well as other species by their smells. Indeed, elephants can been taught to discriminate between the smells of familiar and unfamiliar humans, and to track the smell of an individual.

To summarize, smell information will include where another individual has been, what s/he has been eating, who they are, what their sexual status and age is, who they have been with and so on. In effect, for elephants, the olfactory world could be “something like” reading a newspaper for humans, acquiring information about the past (on the wind, in the water, on the earth, from faeces and urine, from tracks and rubs on vegetation), and possible futures (e.g. oncoming oestrus or a possible birth, movements of groups and forth coming musth). Elephants may also be acquiring information concerning other individual elephants’ (and other animals’) moods (e.g. whether frightened, anxious, or relaxed from body odours such as sweat). Particularly interesting smells are also indicated visually by movements of the trunk.

Gustatory communication

Observations with captive elephants confirm that they have a well developed sense of taste as they taste and smell different or new foodstuffs carefully before eating. One behaviour that was relatively common was placing the trunk in another’s mouth, here tactile, olfactory and taste messages as well as visual, and may be, emotional ones, will be acquired ( further discussion below).

Tactile communication

Despite the thick skin of elephants (5-15mm), they are extremely sensitive to gentle touch. In teaching experiments [58], gently stroking between the front and hind legs and on the shoulder acted as a positive rein forcer, and helps to calm anxious elephants. The multi-sensory trunk is used to discriminate objects by touch [53] and to gather information and communicate to others using all sensory capacities. But, other parts of the body are also used to touch or feel each other, for example, seismic vibrations are picked up through the feet [40,86,87]. The feet are used to search for and manipulate objects (e.g. branches, pieces of rock etc) and feel others. They are also used in violent interactions: To kick, stamp or squash irritants and frightening objects such as fences or other species, including humans. All other parts of the body can also be used to touch, rub, push or scratch another: The head, shoulders, belly, quarters and the ears; as well as the trunk. Eighteen percent of the behaviours in interactions had a tactile component. The common use of tactile communication in this young and predominantly male group may enhance amicable bonds, but further research in family groups is needed.

Auditory communication, audible to the human ear

This was used very little (0.5%) during the observations. In some vocalisations, such as trumpeting, the trunk is used as a vibratory chamber to increase the amplitude of the sound. Trumpeting has sound characteristics which facilitate dispersal and location over a large distance (high amplitude and alternating frequencies facilitating location using the two ears) and trumpets are individually different and recognizable [29], but there were no trumpets during the study. It is possible that the elephants were using infra sound or seismic vibrations, but visual signs of this would be likely and would have been picked up by the observers. Very little research on the occurrence and communicative significance of non vocal noises has been reported in elephants. In this study, “blow” and “puff” accounted for 1% of the behaviours used in social interactions; twice as much as vocalisations and “Blow” appears to have an important communicative value, since it was rarely ignored. Elephants moving around make a variety of noises: Breaking sticks, pushing over trees, pulling branches or grass, sighing, farting and so on, but these were not recorded in this study. When and where these noises are made with intent to communicate (e.g. to stay in touch with the rest of the group) requires further research. However, we know that elephants (and other species) can be aware of making such environmental noises because when they do not want to be found (when hunted or searched for) they can move extremely silently, (indicating there awareness of the noises they make), and stop them intentionally.

Visual communication

All the behaviours recorded and analyzed (with the exception of 0.5% vocal noises) were visual (97%), all be it that some also had olfactory, tactile, gustatory or auditory messages attached. Purely visual signals accounted for over half (58%), it is clear that visual signals are numerous and frequently used in close contact. Further discussion on the use of the trunk and the cause and function of the different visual messages follows.

The trunk, a multi-sensory communicative organ

The elephant’s unique trunk functions as a nose, hand, mouth/tongue, and vibrating chamber, called “synesthesis” [88], as it is used for conveying and receiving messages in all the sensory modalities (Table 4).

The trunk is also a manipulative organ, like humans, elephants are manipulators, humans with their hands, elephants with their trunks. All of their food is collected using the trunk, which sorts, selects, prepares and processes it [24,25,58] .The trunk has approximately 500 muscles controlling its movement [57]. Consequently, it can perform a great variety of movements such as:- Gripping, pushing, twisting, pulling, grabbing, touching. It can be turned and twisted into small places, used to investigate, to caress, push or slap other elephants and humans [59]. It is particularly used to investigate others’ body areas rich in smells, such as the mouth, ears, feet, temporal glands, anus and vulva. It can be used with great skill to lift, pick up, balance, carry, place and stack heavy objects and the tusks, head and trunk working together can act as a fore-loader. Adams [67], personal observations in India with timber elephants). It can also be used to find and lift objects under water (personal observations) and find small hidden objects (such as rings and buckles hidden in hay) and is skilled at sorting objects, even out of sight. It is used for breaking or pushing things over, such as trees or fences (including discriminating between electric and insulated lines, in order to lift posts our of the ground [89]. It is used to make and use tools [89,90] and can be used to manipulate objects in a mirror [91], which humans find difficult. The trunk is used as a resonating cavity for vocalisations, and used to learn to imitate human words [92] and invent new sounds (personal videos elephants in the [93].

The use of the trunk in communication accounted for 18% of all the behaviours in interactions. The 16 different trunk movements had meanings inall 4 of the batched behavioral categories. The trunk is also used of course, for self directed behaviours such as self grooming, smelling, rubbing, scratching, stroking, and essential for drinking, food selection and processing.

The trunk placed in the performer’s own mouth was frequent in training experiments [94], occurring when the individual was uncertain what to do, or confused. It occurs in the same situations in which children put their finger or thumb in their mouths which demonstrate uncertainty [95]. On several occasions when teaching elephants with unprotected contact, when they were frustrated or annoyed, they stamped one front leg. This is a similar behavior given in a similar situation by a child stamping a foot. Beating the trunk on the ground is another well known behaviour in captive elephants associated with frustration (e.g. awaiting food or to be unshackle [51], but was not observed during these observations.

One of the most interesting movements is the trunk placed in another’s mouth, a relatively common behaviour (performed 121x’s by an initiator and by a recipient 54x’s: Total 175x’s). Here the performer acquires information about the recipient and it also demonstrates “trust/confidence/amicability” as biting the other’s trunk would be easy. This action has frequently been observed to have a calming influence on the recipient (observations & videos). Some handlers imitate this action by stroking the tongue of an elephant when greeting. The establishment of predictability in behaviour, “trust” or “confidence in each other” has been widely used by good animal handlers for generations [85,96-98] and is now considered to be one of the most important components in establishing good cooperative animal handling and teaching [58].

The hand/nose/tongue trunk is used in almost every type of situation to manipulate the world, to communicate with others and to read intentions and emotional states of others. Without being aware that his trunk is HIS, the elephant would be unable to do these things, indicating his self awareness of his trunk and his ability to control what, where and when it moves. (Further discussion below).

The indication of parallel experiences between the elephant trunk and human hand underlines the importance of taking a conditional anthropomorphic approach to further comprehend elephant epistemology and mental aptitudes [68,69,71,82].

The meanings of the different behaviours

Communication in elephants, like the rest of their behaviour is flexible and adaptable [24,25,81]. One of the problems is to assess which behaviours (other than vocalisations) are performed with intent to communicate, and which are coincidental and have no communicative significance.

The meanings of many behaviours are self evident; that is, they are “explicit” and can be batched into two categories (i) aggression and avoidance / withdrawing all of which will tend to split the group. (ii) Affiliation and interest in the other, which will encourage group cohesive or sticking. The third category contains behaviours frequently used in communication, which have an “implicit” (inherent, hidden) meaning. Many visual signals are in this category. Further discussion follows.

“Uncertainty”. Behaviours indicative of approach avoidance conflict and frustration, convey a message of indecision

Tail wagging, head shaking and ear flapping are widely used in communication throughout large mammals [26,29,30]. They often occur in approach-avoidance conflict or when frustrated: Where the ability to perform a desired behaviour is inhibited. At such times, often an irrelevant and or less urgent behavior is performed. These have been called “transitional” or “displacement activities” and their original cause related to cutaneous irritation or frustrated movement [26,76,99-101]. Sometimes these behaviours are ritualized for communication (exaggerated and emancipated from their original cause/s) to carry a specific message [76,77,100]. However, highly ritualized displays (which are common in birds), are relatively rare in large herbivores [26], perhaps because the meaning is clearer. For example, behaviours related to skin irritation occur frequently in communication associated with approach-avoidance conflict and frustration [26,78,99,102] and are characterized by some arousal [103]. Behaviours originally associated with cutaneous irritation are tail wagging, ear flapping and head shaking or nodding. Tail wagging was the most frequent; the most common responses were to ignore, or to reciprocate. Ear flapping and head shaking were also frequently ignored or reciprocated. But, since neither head shaking nor ear flapping resulted in aggression, avoidance or withdrawal, they do not indicate any threat, but rather that the communicant is slightly aroused and indecisive/ uncertain what to do. 

An interesting behaviour in the same category is placing the trunk in his own mouth. Children may also place their fingers in their moths when in indecisive situations [26,29,58,95], another similarity to humans. 

Anselme & Gurturkun [104], in a recent paper shows that “uncertainty” magnifies food searching behavior in birds which then increases exploration and survival. This “uncertainty principle” could equally be applied in communication to reduce potentially emotionally inflammatory situations, encourage group cohesion which in turn will encourage social exploration and curiosity, leading to the acquisition of more environmental and social knowledge.

Ignoring directed behaviours

Ignoring a directed behaviour accounted for 30% of the recipients responses. The reasons for ignoring could be that the directed behaviour had no meaning, or, that the recipient had not seen it. Since all the performed behaviours were responded to on other occasions, ignoring cannot be the result of it having no meaning. Since the initiators behavior was only recorded when it was clearly directed at another, it is unlikely that the recipient was not aware of it as elephants are visual aware as we have shown (Table 2).

Therefore, the most likely explanation is that the recipient chose to ignore that behaviour directed at him/her. Detailed investigation of what behaviours were ignored indicates that some of the most frequently ignored demonstrated “uncertainty” (e.g. tail wag, head nod, flap ears, swing trunk and head shake). The message of “uncertainty” or indecision had been received, but, because there was no threat to social cohesion, there was no need for a response. Ignoring in this situation may help to deflate any emotional conflict.

It is less clear why social behaviours, such as approach and contact make, turn body towards, turn head towards, tail up (indicating a rise in excitement), trunk up, and tusking another’s body are so frequently ignored. But this may indicate that ignoring a situation in which conflicts might otherwise arise and grow, will aid group cohesion.

Behaviours that are less ignored will have strong messages. These include putting the trunk in the mouth of another, which shows interest, familiarity, confidence or “trust”. “Blow” and trunk extended sniffing were also rarely ignored both indicating something like” take heed, there is something interesting around”. Drawing both ears back against the head was also infrequently ignored; which may have a message something like “I am slightly concerned about the situation” and the recipient reciprocating message will mean: “I have seen you and am therefore also slightly concern” .

Do as you have been done by/ reciprocity or imitation

Another puzzle is that reciprocity/ imitative / ‘do as you have been done by’/‘be done by as you did’ [105], is much more frequent than expected overall and for particular behaviours. This is relevant to discussions concerning mirror neurons [106,107], which have now been demonstrated to be widely distributed in different species rather than restricted to humans. 

Reciprocity and its role in fostering bonding and cooperation has been discussed in some animals [108], but has not previously been reported in elephants. Sixteen of the most commonly recorded behaviours all of which demonstrated affection/sticking behaviour or uncertainty, were very significantly imitated by the recipient. These were those involving orientation movements and postures (ears prick or turn head towards the other,) investigation (trunk up & smelling), interest in another (approach and contact make). They included those indicating an increase in arousal and preparation for action such “tail up” [26]. Behaviours indicating “uncertainty”/ indecision, were also significantly reciprocated (such as tail wag, head nod, swing trunk, head shake and trunk to own mouth).

It may be that reciprocal behaviour is more common than has been previously reported in many species. Randle & Kiley-Worthington showed that inter-species imitation (the animal imitating a novel action of the human teacher) is frequent in teaching situations and, as in children, can be used as a teaching aid. The function may be to indicate to another; something like “I am also feeling the same way”, that is emotional contagion [65,66], which is another way of indicating awareness of others’ feelings and intentions. It is interesting that elephants can also be taught to imitate humans speech which is suggested helps them to bond with humans [92]. 

Kingsley [105], maintains that both “do as you would be done by “that is be nice to each other, and “be done by as you did” (if nasty, it will be returned) are moral rules which children should obey. Following on from this, reciprocating with aggressive behaviours when aggressed would ensure that the social contract is adhered to and those who do not adher can leave the society, so the society can continue. Reciprocity of behaviour (and its accompanying emotions) that might threaten the group stability and the social contract: “Be done by as you did”, would then discourage further contravention of the social contract.

Context dependent and context independent meanings

Context dependent meanings for some communication in elephants (and other species) has been demonstrated for many vocalizations [27], and head and tail movements in ungulates and carnivores [26,30].

One of the particularly interesting consequences of context dependent messaging is that, because a particular behaviour acts as an analogue system to convey a message about the general level of excitement or arousal of the individual, many behaviours are used in a variety of situations. The specific meaning, that is why the elephant is aroused, can only be gathered from the context, thus whether s/he is frightened, worried, feeling slightly aggressive or sexy will only be gathered from the context. As a result, each individual must be acutely aware of the environment and the intent of others or he will not be able to interpret accurately the meaning of the message. Any witness will have to have an understanding of the situation and be aware of others’ intentions that are his emotions, desires and needs to interpret the message. Such a reading of intentions of others has been labeled having a “theory of mind”: Defined as “attributing perceptual, epistemic, and volitional states to others and themselves” [109-117]. Therefore, animals that use context dependent communication must have a ‘theory of mind’ [71,110] or they would not be able to communicate, have a social contract and a social organization. 

By contrast, to understand the meaning of context independent messages (such as human language), it is not always necessary to have an awareness of others intentions, desires and needs because the meaning of the message is constant, whatever the situation, emotions and intentions of the communicant which is what allows human language to be understood out of the present context: On the computer or telephone. This is because context independent communication conveys complex, symbolic non-emotional messages entirely divorced from the context and, consequently, can be understood without having any theory of mind of the communicator and his intentions. 

Context dependent communication therefore presents a different complex cognitive assessment of the world from context independent language. But, judgments concerning mental development and complexity, are usually made only by comparison to humans, often because of the overruling importance of context independent language in which humans both speak and think most of the time which acts as a cognitive handicap when attempting to understanding the ontology and epistemology of species who use context dependent communication and who, to understand the message clearly, must be acutely aware of the social and physical environment around them and the feelings and intentions of others [66,71]. Without an awareness of others feelings and intentions, such communication will not function and without communications, there could be no social contract, recognition of other individuality, and their knowledge or roles, that is no “social networking” [11].

Rethinking elephants social contract and their epistemology related to these results

It may be that unless there is a need for constant competition within a group, (such as access to food, shelter or other scarce resources to ensure survival), the basis of any social grouping in mammals is emotional attachments [81,118,119], rather than competition for resources. This ontologically starts with the mother/infant attachment [120], controlled initially hormonally but as a result of learning, often reinforced and continuing into adulthood. Thus, it is reasonable to consider that positive emotional bonds become the social cement of some societies [121], rather than socially negative or competitive behaviours which will tend to disperse or split the group and “dominance hierarchies” evolved to reduce competition and injury to group members. The “dominance hierarchy” gives priority of access to resources in all situations to certain individuals. But, the explanation of elephant society in terms of “dominance hierarchies” [73,74,122], has obscured the importance of less melodramatic, non competitive behaviours including those that could help to de flame potentially inflammatory situations and split the group, such as ignoring or indecisive behaviours. 

In addition in species such as large herbivours where resources are usually widespread and available for all or none, there is little need for constant competition within the group. Individuals have different personalities and consequently different placings in the different behavioural hierarchies, that is some are more aggressive than others, some more affiliative. Some more submissive, and some more socially involved [85]. But, there is not one individual who is top of the aggressive hierarchy and low in the avoidance hierarchy as there would be if there was a “dominance hierarchy” controlling access to resources. The organization of this society is more complex with individuals having a variety of different roles.

In large free living herbivors since there is no need for constant competition for resources, behaviours and their accompanying emotions which encourage group cohesion would be likely to be common and selected for such as those demonstrating interest and affiliation to keep the group together. Demonstrations of indecision that is “uncertainty”, “reciprocity” and ignoring directed behaviour may also help to here the group by reducing negative emotions and possible group splitting.

The question remains why group cohesion is so important. To grow, survive and reproduce, each individual must become a natural botanist, zoologist, ethologist, meteologist, geologist and geographer that is s/he must become a good “natural ecologist” [81,123]. There is an enormous amount of information to acquire by each infant in order to survive, so perhaps, the most important reason for group living is to enable the acquisition of such necessary knowledge by facilitating observational, social learning and imitation, which aid in a rapid and less risky accumulation of knowledge than learning everything by trial and error. However, it is necessary to know who to learn from: To know what knowledge other individual have. Thus an awareness of the age, personality, role [124] and knowledge of each group member will be crucial. Thus, older animal’s greater knowledge is recognized so they often become decision makers or “matriarchs” [25,71]. Strong, askilled manipulators will have a role of helping infants out of mud holes [125,126].

If the society is to prosper and reproduce, the society’s rules, that is, the social contract must be obeyed. Such rules as “do not attack infants”, ”recognize others roles” or “do not perform socially negative behavior unless another breaks the rules”. If an individual does break the rules, then action must be taken. This would account for the frequency of reciprocal behaviours: “If you are nice to me, I will be nice to you; if you are nasty, I will return it in kind”.

There are “other ways of seeing” outlined by Goethe [127], which may be imperative when considering other species epistemology. One idea that is becoming increasingly apparent and may be one of the most significant and important developments for life, is the recognition of how wide spread symbiosis is, both between and within species [128-130]. The importance of symbiosis in the social world of different species [131,132] and different strategies to encourage this in the social contract is another way forward to an understanding of that species epistemology [66,71, 81,82].

The question remains why, what would appear to be a relatively small evolutionary step, that of associating a particular movement posture or call with a particular message, has not always been taken. But, different cognitive maps are held by different individuals and species, for example blind humans become more sensitive to wind, smells and touch than normal humans, and other “handicaps” in humans also point out different cognitive maps. For different species, an emphasis on olfaction may require a different concept of space and time from that of humans [81]. A different world view will be acquired by two simultaneous large monocular visual fields, compared to a large binocular visual field and poor peripheral monocular vision characteristic of humans. The different world view implicated by context dependent communication also illustrates a different world view since when using context dependent communication, the recipient has to be aware of the “perceptual epistemic, volitional states of the other” to receive the correct message and must “socially network”. 

There is much evidence that non-human mammals do recognize other living “beings”; that is they recognize the body/mind/moods/emotions/intentions/knowledge/roles of others, both inter- and intra-species, and discriminate between the living and the dead [25]. Such communication requires an awareness of others body/mind being that context independent communication does not always need. Thus, awareness that the other has a mind is a necessary attribute for context dependent communication.

Many even naïve/wild mammals have an ability to read others feelings and intentions, including those of other mammals, humans. Even naïve rhinos, horses, dogs, cattle, elephants, lions, tigers respond rapidly to humans moods, although some humans may need experience before they are able to read the intentions of an animal [58]. Having an awareness of others and their intentions is a necessary mental attribute for context dependent language users without which they could not have a social ontology, social organizations or a social network. Whether such animals have “reflective self-knowledge” [133-138], is not the same question.

CONCLUSION

Studying communication of different species in detail can throw a more distinctive light on another species social contract and consequently their epistemology/ world view. Much of elephant communication is visual, and uses several senses at once, where as vocal communication (audible to the human ear) was relatively uncommon in this group. Much interactive behaviour is ignored, but may also be reciprocated more than expected. There are also behaviours whose meaning is implicit but on examination, these behaviours often indicate “uncertainty” which can de flame a potentially emotionally inflammable situation.

Correlations of the individuals rankings in the different batched behaviours, did not confirm that there is any overall “dominant” individual, but that individuals have different roles in the society and that the prime society parameter is to facilitate it’s cohesion: “Sticking behaviour”, not competitive or “splitting behaviour” where some priority of access for an individual that is a “dominance hierarchy” might be the most important organizational parameter. 

Positive or “sticking” behavior (affiliative and showing interest in another), are much more frequently than expected which indicates that, for this large herbivore, the cohesion of the group is more important than competition within it (“splitting” behavior).Cohesive and symbiotic behavior, may have been selected for to facilitate knowledge transfer by social and observational learning.

The meaning of much of elephant communication is context dependent that is the same behaviours are performed in a variety of situations. To interpret the specific meaning of a message, the recipient must be aware of the situation and the intention of others. Judgments concerning mental attributes only by comparison to humans discourage consideration of different and complex social ontology’s and can be a cognitive handicap, we should broaden the search in order to better understand a species epistemology. Animals think, and we can begin to understand what different species think about [139,140].

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

The owners of Imire Safari Park, Zimbabwe, the Travers family are thanked for allowing us to work on their elephants and providing accommodation. All the elephant handlers are thanked for their enthusiastic help with collecting the data, Chum, Morris, Peter & Dunmore. Hayley Randle is thanked for helping with the field work and doing much of the original analysis. The animals at the Eco-Etho Research Centre provided the rest of the finance.

FUNDING

This research did not receive any specific grant funding from agencies in the public community or form the non-profit sector.

REFERENCES

  1. Hart BL, Hart L (2007) Evolution of the Elephant Brain: A Paradox between Brain Size and Cognitive Behavior. Evolution of Nervous Systems 3: 491-497.
  2. Jerison HJ (1973) Evolution of brain size and intelligence. Acedemic Press, New York, London, UK.
  3. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 4436-4441.
  4. Shoshani J, Kupsky WJ, Marchant GH (2006) Elephant brain. Part I: gross morphology, functions, comparative anatomy, and evolution. Brain Res Bull 70: 124-157.
  5. Roth G, Dicke U (2005) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in cognitive sciences 9: 250-257.
  6. Bryne RTW, Corp N (2004) Neocortex size predictions deception rate in primates. Proc Bio Sci 271: 1693-1699.
  7. McGregor PK, Peake TM (2000) Communication networks: Social environments for receiving and signalling behaviour. acta ethologica 2: 71-81.
  8. Peake TM, Terry AMR, McGregor PK, Dabelsteen T (2001) Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male–to–male vocal interactions. Proc Royal Soc 268: 1472.
  9. McGregor PK, Horn AG (2015) Communication and social networks. In: Krause J, James R, Franks DW, Croft DP (eds.). Animal social networks. Oxford University Press, London, UK.
  10. Emery NJ, Clayton NS, Frith CR (2007) Introduction. Social intelligence: From brain to culture. Phil Trans R Soc B 362: 489-505.
  11. Krause J, James R, Franks D, Croft D (2015) Animal Social Networks. Oxford University Press, London, UK.
  12. Ridley R (2016) Peter Pan and the Mind of J M Barrie. An Exploration of Cognition and Consciousness. UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, UK.
  13. Conner RC (2007) Dolphin social intelligence: Complex alliance relationships in bottlenose dolphins and a consideration of selective environments for extreme brain size evolution in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 362: 587-602.
  14. Chomsky N (1972) Language and Mind, Harcourt Brace Jovanovitch, New York, USA
  15. Carruthers P (1992) The animals issue. Moral theory in practice. Cambridge Univ Press,USA.
  16. Searle JR (1994) The rediscovery of the mind, Representation and Mind series, MIT, London, UK.
  17. Zuberbuhler K (2010) Evolution of mammalian vocal signals: Development of semiotic content and semantics of human language in Handbook of Neuroscience. Chapter 11: 505-513.
  18. Gardner RA, Gardner BT (1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165: 664-672.
  19. Hockett CF (1960) Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. American Institute of Biological Sciences, Pg no: 39.
  20. Lilly JC (1961) Man and Dolphin. Pyramid Books, Gollanz London, UK.
  21. Hermann LM (1987) Receptive competencies of language trained. Advances in the Study of Behavior 17: 1-60.
  22. Savage-Rumbaugh S, Brakke KE (1996) Animal language: Methodological and interpretive issues. In: Allen C, Jamison D (eds.). Readings in Animal Cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA.
  23. Anderson SR (2004) Doctor Dolittle’s Delusion. Yale Univ Press, USA.
  24. Douglas-Hamilton IO (1975) Among the Elephants. Collins, London. Pg no: 285.
  25. Moss CJ (1988) Elephant Memories: Thirteen Years in the Life of an Elephant Family. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.
  26. Kiley-Worthington M (1976) The tail movements of ungulates, canids and felids with particular reference to causation and their function as displays. Behav 56: 69-115.
  27. Wood JD, Mccowan B, Langauber WR, Viljoen JJ, Heart LA (2006) Classification of African elephant Loxodonta Africana rumbles using acoustic parameters and cluster analysis. Bioacoustics Intern J Animal Sound & Its Recording 15: 143-161.
  28. Heffner RS, Heffner HE (1982) Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maximus): absolute sensitivity, frequency discrimination, and sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol 96: 926-944.
  29. Kiley M (1969) The displays of carnivores and ungulates with particular relation to their origin and evolution. D.Phil University of Sussex, UK.
  30. Kiley M (1972) The vocalisations of ungulates. Their cause and function. Z Tierpsychol 31: 171-222.
  31. 31. Clemins PJ, Johnson MT (2003) Application of speech recognition to african elephant (Loxodonta africana) vocalisations. IEEE International Conference 1: 484-487.
  32. Solstis J, Leong A, Savage A (2005) African elephant vocal communication II: rumble variation reflects the individual identity and emotional state of callers. Animal Behav 70: 589-599.
  33. Payne KB, Thompson M, Kramer L (2003) Elephant calling patterns as indicators of group size and composition: the basis for an acoustic monitoring system. African J Ecology 41: 99-107.
  34. Poole JH, Payne K, Langbauer WR Jr, Moss CJ (1988) The social contexts of some very low frequency calls of African elephants. Behav Ecol & Sociobiol 22: 385-392.
  35. Leong KM, Ortolani A, Graham LH, Savage A (2003) The use of low-frequency vocalizations in African elephant (Loxodonta africana) reproductive strategies. Horm Behav 43: 433-443.
  36. McComb K, Reby D, Baker L, Moss C, Sayialel S (2003) Long-distance communication of acoustic cues to social identity in African elephants. Anim Behav 65: 317-329.
  37. Garstang M (2004) Long-distance, low-frequency elephant communication. J Comp Phys & Neuroethol, Sensory Neural & Behav Physiology 190: 791-805.
  38. Clemins PJ, Johnson MT, Leong KM, Savage A (2005) Automatic classification and speaker identification of african elephant (Loxodonata africana) vocalisations. J Acoustical Soc of Americ 117: 956-963.
  39. Poole JH, Tyack PL, Stoeger-Horwath AS, Watwood S (2005) Animal behaviour: Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature 435: 455-456.
  40. Gunther RH, O’Connell-Rodwell CE, Klemperer SL (2004) Seismic waves for elephant vocalisations: A possible communication mode? Geophysical Research Letters 31: L11602.
  41. O’Conell-Rodwell CE, Wood JD, Rodwell TC, Puri S, PartanSR, et al. (2006) Wild elephant (Loxodonta africana) breeding herds respond to artificially transmitted seismic stimuli. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 59: 842-850.
  42. Mortimer B, Rees WL, Koelemeijer P, Nissen-Meyer T (2018) Classifying elephant behaviour through seismic vibrations. Current Bio 28: 587-548.
  43. Hakeem AY, Hof PR, Sherwood CC, Switzer RC 3rd, Rasmussen LE, et al. (2005) Brain of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana): Neuroanatomy from magnetic resonance images. Anat Rec A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol 287: 1117-1127.
  44. Watson L (1999) The Organ of Jacobson Allen Lane. Penguin. London, United Kingdom.
  45. Gobbel L, Fischer MS, Smith TD, Wibler JR, Bhatnager KP (2004) The vomeronasal organ and associated structures of the fetal African elephant Loxodonta africana (Proboscidea, Elephantindae). Acta Zoologica 85: 41-52.
  46. Rasmussen LE, Schmidt MJ, Henneous R (1982) Asian bull elephants flehmen -like response to extracted compounds of female estrus urine. Sci 4555: 159-162.
  47. Dehnhard M, Heistermann M, Geritz F, Hermes R, Hildebrandt T, et al. (2001) Demonstration of volatile C19-steroids in the urine of female Asian elephants Elephas maximus, and their dependence on ovarian activity. Reproduction 121: 475-484.
  48. Goodwin TE, Rasmussen LEI, Schulte BA, Brown PA, Davis BL, et al. (2005) Chemical analysis of ovulatory female african elephant urine: A search for putative pheromones: In: Mason Rt, LeMaster MP, Mueller Schwarze D (eds.). In Chemical signals in vertebrates 10: 128-139.
  49. Lamps LW, Smoller, BR, Goodwin TE, Rasmussen LEL (2004) Hormone receptor expression in interdigital glands of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Zoo biology 23: 463-469.
  50. Schulte BA, Bagley K, Correll M, Gray A, Heinman SM, et al. (2005) Assessing chemical communication in elephants. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10: 140-151.
  51. Carrington R (1958) Elephants Penguin. London, United Kingdom.
  52. Stanley L, Goodwin TE, Rasmussen LEL (2005) Assessing chemical communication in elephants in Chemical signals in vertebrates. In: Mason RT, LeMaster MP, Mueller. 10: 140-151, New York.
  53. Makecha R, Fad O, Kuczaj II SA (2012) The role of touch in the social interactions of Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). International Journal of Comparative Psychology 25: 60-82.
  54. McFarland DJ (1996) On the causal and functional significance of displacement activities. Z Tierpsychol 23: 217-235.
  55. Vidya TN, Sukumar R (2005) Social organization of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) in southern India inferred from microsatellite DNA. Journal of Ethology 23: 205-210.
  56. Yokoyama S, Takenaka N, Agnew DW, Shoshani J (2005) Elephants and human color-blind deuteranopes have identical sets of visual pigments. Genetics 170: 335-344.
  57. Redmond I (1993) Elephants Dorling Kindersley. London, UK.
  58. Kiley-Worthington M, Rendle-Worthington J (2012) Exploding the Myths: Mammal Welfare, Handling and Teaching. Ex Libris, London, UK.
  59. Baze W (1955) Just Elephants trans M. M. Burton. Corgi Books. London, United Kingdom.
  60. Estes RD (1991) The behavior guide to African mammals russell friedman. Halfway House. South Africa.
  61. Plotnik JM, Shaw RC, Brubaker DL, Tiller LN, Clayton NS (2014) Thinking with their trunks: elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude non rewarding alternatives. Anim Behav 88: 91-98.
  62. Kahl MP, Armstong BD (2002) Visual displays of wild African elephants during musth. Mammalia 66: 159-171.
  63. Rasmussen LEL, Greenwood DR (2005) Reproduction in Asian elephants has behavioural and biochemical foundations. in Chemical Sense 8.
  64. Kiley-Worthington M, Randle H (1997) Animal educational psychology. A comparative study of teaching 4 mammals of different species. Eco Research Centre.
  65. Hatfield E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL (1993) Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2: 96-99.
  66. de Waal (2016) Are we smart enough to know how smart animals are? Norton, New York.
  67. Adams J (1981) Wild elephants in captivity. Elephant training procedures: Step by step methods formerly kept secret by trainers throughout the world. Carson California. The Center for the Study of Elephants,USA.
  68. Fisher JA (1991) Disambiguating anthropomorphism: An inter-disciplinary review. Perspectives in Ethology. Plenum Press, 124-35.
  69. Burghardt JM (1991) Cognitive Ethology and Critical Anthropomorphism. A snake with two heads and hog-nosed snakes that plays dead. in: Cognitive Ethologyed: C.A.Ristau. Erlbaum. Hillsdale BvKenya 1954 An Elephant Hunter in South Africa Penguin. Capetown.
  70. Welmensfelder F (2008) Curiosity, sentience, integrity: Why recognizing that the whole animal matters. David Wood-Gush Memorial Lecuture International Soc Applied Ethology, Dublin.
  71. Kiley-Worthington M (2017) The mental homologies of mammals. towards an understanding of another mammals world view. Animals (Basel) 7: 12.
  72. Silk JB, A Samuels, PS Rodman (1981) The influence of kinship, rank and sex on affilation and aggression between adult female and immature bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Behav 78: 111-137.
  73. Archie EA, Morrison TA, Foley CAH, Moss CJ, Alberts SC (2006) Dominance rank relationships among wild female african elephants, Loxodonta africana. Animal Behaviour 71: 117-127.
  74. Wittemeyer G, Getz WM (2007) Hierarchical dominance structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Animal Behaviour 73: 671-681.
  75. Candiotti A, Zumbeurhler K, Lemasson A (2012) Context-related call combinations in female Diana moneys. J Animal cognition 15: 327-339.
  76. Morris D (1957) “Typical intensity” and its relation to the problem of ritualisation. Behav 11: 1-12.
  77. Huxley JH (1960) Royal Society. London, UK.
  78. Berlyne DG (1960) Conflict, Arousal and Curiosity. McGraw Hill. New York, USA.
  79. Corr PJ (2013) Approach avoidance behavior: Multiple systems and their interaction. Emotional Review 3: 285-290.
  80. Eder AB, Elliot AJ, Harmon-Jones E (2013) Approach avoidance motivation: Issues and advances. Emotion review 5: 227-229.
  81. Kiley-Worthington M (2000) Equine and elephant epistemology, Right in front of your mind. M. Phil thesis, University of Lancaster, UK.
  82. Kiley-Worthington M (2011) A comparative study of equine and elephant mental attributes leading to an acceptance of their subjectivity and consciousness. J Consciousness Exploration & Research 2: 10-50.
  83. Randle H, Kiley-Worthington M (1994) Factors facilitating the breeding of black rhino in semi-captivity. Abs. Appled Ethology, full paper Eco Research Centre occasional paper 005.
  84. Altman J (1974) Observational study of behaviour. Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227-267.
  85. Kiley-Worthington M (2005) Horse Watch: What it is to be Equine. J A Allen, London, UK.
  86. Kenya Wildlife Service in Laikipia (1998) consultancy on elephants manipulating fences. Kenya Wildlife Service in Laikipia, Nanyuki, Kenya.
  87. Hill PSM (2001) Vibration and animal communication: A review. Integrative and Comparative Biology 41: 1135-1142.
  88. Ternaux JP (2006) Synesthesia: A Multimodal Combination of Senses. Project MUSE, Muse 36: 321-322.
  89. Kiley-Worthington M (1990) The behaviour of animals in zoos and circuses. Chiron’s world? Oriel. Stockton, California, USA.
  90. Nassani M (2006) Do Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) apply causal reasoning to tool-use tasks? J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32: 91-96.
  91. Povinelli DJ (1989) Failure to Find Self-Recognition in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in contrast to their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. J Compa Pscyhol 103: 122-131.
  92. Stoeger AS, Mietchen D, Oh S, de Silva S, Herbst CT, et al. (2012) An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Curr Bio 22: 2144-2148.
  93. (2016) Groves elephants in the Okovongo delta. Botswana
  94. Randle H, Kiley-Worthington M (1997) Social relations in a small group of African elephants. (Loxondonta africana). Eco Research Centre, occas paper 008.
  95. Jacobs TJ (1994) Nonverbal communications: Some reflections on their role in the psychoanalytic process and psychoanalytic education. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 42: 741-762.
  96. Pojansky A (1997) My horses my teachers, AbeBooks, Victoria, Canada.
  97. Karl P (2008) Twisted truths of modern dressage. Cadmos Richmond UK.
  98. Wilson DAH (2015) The welfare of performing animals. A historical perspective. Animals 6: 76.
  99. Andrews RJ (1963) The origin and evolution of the calls and facial expressions of the primates. Behaviour 20: 1-109.
  100. Tinbergen N (1956) The study of Instinct. OUP, Oxford, UK.
  101. Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. (2nd edn). Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
  102. Bindra D (1959) Motivation: A systematic reinterpretation. Ronald Press, New York, USA.
  103. Smith JD (2005) Studies of uncertainty monitoring and metacognition in animals and humans. In Terrace, Metcalf (eds.). The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. OUP Oxford, Oxford, UK.
  104. Anselme P, Gunturkun O (2018) How foraging works. Uncertainty magnifies food seeking motivation. Behav Brain Sci 8: 1-106.
  105. Kingsley C (1880) The water babies. Ulwencreutz Media, London, UK.
  106. Keysers C (2009) Mirror neurons. Current Biology 19: 971-973
  107. Kilner JM, Lemon RN (2013) What we know currently about mirror neurons. Curr Biol 23: 1057-1062.
  108. Friedin E, Carballo F, Bentosela M (2015) Direct reciprocity in animals: The roles of bonding and affective processes. Int J Psychol 52: 163-170.
  109. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav & Brain Sci 1: 515-526.
  110. Terrace HS, Metcalfe J (2005) The missing link in cognition. Origins of self-reflective consciousness. OUP. Oxford, UK.
  111. Tulving E (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesis. Uniquely Human? In: Terrace HS, Metcalf J. The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. OUP, Oxford, UK.
  112. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrib jays. Nature 395: 272-274.
  113. Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A (2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 683-691.
  114. Kinsbourne M (2005) A continuum of self-consciousness that emerges in phylogeny and ontogeny. In: Terrace HS, Metcalf J (eds.). The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, USA.
  115. Call J, Tomesello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind 30 years later? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 187-192.
  116. Damasio AR (1994) Descartes error. Vintage books, New York,USA.
  117. Blackmore S (2003) Consciousness and introduction. Hodder & Stoughton. London, United Kingdom.
  118. Darwin C (1868) The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London, United Kingdom.
  119. Bernstein IS (1981) Dominance: The baby and the bathwater Behav. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4: 419-429.
  120. Midgely M (1978) Beast and man: The roots of human nature. Cornell press, Ithaca, New York, USA.
  121. Goleman D (1996) Emotional intelligence. Bloomsbury, London, United Kingdom.
  122. Jameson KA, Appleby MC, Freeman LC (1999) Finding an appropriate order for the hierarchy based on probabilistic dominance. Anim Behav 57: 991-998.
  123. Avital E, Jablonka E (2000) Animal traditions, behaioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge, USA, Pg no: 432.
  124. Garland JS (1968) Structure and function in primate society. Folia Primatol (Basel) 8: 89-120.
  125. Animal Planet, BBC TV Series shown on the 22.02.2007
  126. Bates LA, Lee PC, Njuraubu N, Poole JH, Sayialel K, et al. (2008) Elephants show empathy? Journal of Consciousness Studies 15: 204-225.
  127. Bortoft H (1996) The wholeness of natur, Goethe’s way of science. Floris Books, Edinburgh, United Kingdom.
  128. Margulis L, Sagan D (2008) Acquiring Genomes: A theory of the origin of species. Hachette UK, London, UK.
  129. Tsing A, Swanson H, Gan H, Bubandt N (2017) The art of living on a damaged planet. University of Minnesota Press. USA.
  130. Gilbert SF, Epel D (2015) Ecological developmental biology Sunderland Mass. Sinauer Assoc.
  131. Jolly A (2015) Thank you, Madagascar. Zed books, The University of Chicago Press Books, Chicago, USA.
  132. Haraway DJ (1989) Primate visions: Gender, race and nature in the world of modern science. NY Routledge, New York, USA.
  133. O’Shaughnessy B (2000) Consciousness and the world. Oxford University Press, Oxford, England, UK.
  134. Roessler J (2004) Review of Consciousness and the World B. O’Shaughnessy. Brit J Phil Sci 55: 163-173.
  135. Smith JD (2009) The study of animal metacognition. Trends Cogn Sci 13: 389-396.
  136. Kiley-Worthington M (1987) The behaviour of horses: In relation to management and training J A Allen, London, UK.
  137. Greenwood DR, Comeskey D, Hunt MB, Rasmussen EL (2005) Chemical Communication: Chirality in Elephant pheromones. Nature 438: 1097-1098.
  138. Schulte BA, Bagley K, Correll M, Gray A, Heineman SM, et al. (2005) Chemical communication in elephants. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10: 140-151.
  139. Lea S, Kiley-Worthington M (1996) Can animals think? In: Bruce V (ed.). Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition. American Psychological Association, Oxford, UK. Pg no: 211-244
  140. Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, et al. (2010) Prevalence, clinical correlates and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry 60: 991-997.

Citation: Kiley-Worthington M (2019) Communication in a Small Herd of Semi-Domestic Elephants another Interpretation of their Social Organization, Social Contract and World View. J Anim Res Vet Sci 3: 012.

Copyright: © 2019  Marthe Kiley-Worthington, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.


Herald Scholarly Open Access is a leading, internationally publishing house in the fields of Sciences. Our mission is to provide an access to knowledge globally.



© 2024, Copyrights Herald Scholarly Open Access. All Rights Reserved!